Monday, November 23, 2015
How Would Real Capitalism Work?
The derivatives market is twenty times the total world economy. At $1.2 quadrillion the derivatives market is undefinable, complex, unregulated, and highly profitable. Derivates are a complex set of investments that are based on future events. For example if i bet that someone who has aides is likely to die and then I can buy their life insurance policy earlier and make a profit that is a derivative transaction. But more importantly, if I take action to make sure that that person does not live longer (by restricting life-extending medications, or restricting access to information that might delay their demise, then such action would increase the value of my derivatives. Then there are those in the "positive feedback loop" that just follow the market as it goes down and sell a proportion of their stock and hopefully countered by "negative feedback loop" that buy when it goes low and sell when high.
Saturday, November 21, 2015
We are Becoming Gods
At one of the seminars at the University of Melbourne students were discussing prostitution (which is legal in Melbourne), I was wondering why we find certain activities distasteful: Defecating, getting drunk, binge eating, spitting, drooling, burping, public sex, masturbation, dying, giving birth, nursing a baby, crying, farting, being needy…and a pattern started to emerge. These behaviors are natural—even mechanical—aspect of our biological being, and the only reason that we find these activities distasteful is because there must be an imbedded ideal standard that we aspire to.
In our mind we have a model of the world. The sole purpose of having such a complex brain is to represent the world in its entirety as far as it affects us. Every day we adjust this model to make it closer to reality. An unachievable objective since reality is ephemeral but we make reality conform to patterns that help us to predict it. Both our dreams and our waking emotions signal a need to modify and adjust this view of the world. And this cognitive representation remains mostly unconscious. Our brain interacts unconsciously first and only gives us consciousness when it requires our full attention in order to address some complex event. This is the internal world that is driving us to think like gods. This unconscious model of the world furnishes us with a feeling of mastery and control because we can predict and affect change. But this feeling of mastery is an illusion and it is this illusion that is growing.
Rousseau's ‘Malo periculosam libertatem quam quietum servitium” If gods were people, they would govern themselves democratically. In our cognitive model the world is predictable and just (Lerner, 1980). Despite an onslaught of daily news informing us otherwise, we still believe in a just world. We continue to be surprised by disasters or catastrophes thinking that they are exceptions. They are not. They are exceptions only in our model of the world—the one that we cultivate in our head, the virtual box—because everything is in harmony, everything is balanced, and just. We continue to aspire to a world where we can "cure" death, "regain" our youth, "fight" terrorism, "save" humanity...these are illogical and delusional aims only if you are NOT a god. If we aspire to behave like—or think that we are—gods, then these aspirations are attainable. These aspirations confer a delusional sense of control over our world.
The Emergence of Individuality
The idea that we are somewhat godlike requires that we have a belief that we are unique individuals. Jean Twenge and Keith Campbell exploration of the narcissistic epidemic (Twenge & Campbell, 2009) document an alarming rise of narcissism at every level of our society. With the pampering of social media promoting a world filled with egos, our individualistic selves are thriving. We only have to look at the world economy where such individualistic arrogance is promoted by banks, despite resulting in risky, unrealistic investments. A financial system that enshrines the motto that it is "too big to fail". Godlike structures which are resilient to reality and to change, that rest above the law and it seems above basic economics. And such arrogance comes across from individuals running these structures. Among the oligarch—the very rich—there has always been godlike arrogance. Traditionally oligarch were philanthropic, they wanted to change the world for the better. But it is their view of “better” that is creating inequity. Not only are we turning into a world—not just a society—run by oligarchs, but we are seeing these inequities transferred across generations. The oligarchs have discovered immortality they are transferring their wealth down successive generations. Their individualism will live into eternity though the management of their wealth. These are not delusions of grandeur, since these individuals have the power to affect great social changes. And oligarchs have always been with us, although perhaps not to such as extent as today. What is uniquely transformative is the emerging belief that anyone can have this power. This form of individualism has infected the general public. And it is this belief—that anyone can become an oligarch—allows us to sell short our collective futures.
Individualism should not be confused with individuality. Individuality is a psychological concept of self that is separate and distinct from others. Individualism is a historical interpretation of self as the center of all interest and a belief that individual achievement rather than community or societal progress is the ultimate development. That the moral and intellectual imperative resides with the individual and not the community, something that is now shared with corporations (cf. Lukes 1990). Individualism is a deformity of individuality. There is a certain form, a shape that individuality takes that fits in within society. But through individualism, this shape become malformed, and no longer fits within the bigger puzzle of society. It stands out as separate and, in some cases, an antithesis to the social setting it resides in. Individualism is predicated on the expectations that wellbeing and life satisfaction are achieved only through one’s personal goals rather than through community achievements (Diener and Diener 1995). An offshoot of this is that we base our predictions—judgment, reasoning and causal inference—based on the person (or people) rather than the situation or social context (Morris and Peng, 1994). We judge others, and not the context that we find them in. Terrorists are deranged people rather than sane people in deranged contexts. Our godlike behavior elevates control of behavior to internal thought rather than social contexts.
There is evidence that in pre-history—before written records—humans were aware of their individuality but without individualism. Historically our personality was shared with the community that we lived in. Bell provides numerous examples where “there is wide agreement among anthropologists, evolutionists and cognitive specialists that early humans had little or no awareness of themselves as independent personalities, but instead felt themselves to be parts of the group (collective) to which they belonged.” (Bell, 2010) Remnants of this common sharing is still seen in marriage rituals were the marriage is seen as uniting two families together, in as much as a coupling of two individuals.
The growth of individualism has been identified in psychiatry through the work on personality. Personality is a hypothetical entity that cannot be observed or studied other than when studied within interpersonal situations. There is no “I” in personality unless there is an interaction with others. The “I” without a social interpersonal context does not exist. In Social Identity Theory 'individuals define themselves in terms of their social group memberships and that group-defined self-perception produces psychologically distinctive effects in social behaviour' (Turner, 1982). This socialization is what makes us distinct. As early as the 1950s, Harry Sullivan in “The Illusion of Individuality” argued that: “…human beings are human animals that have been filled with culture—socialized…” (p. 323) His personality theory is based on relationships rather than internal psychodynamics (such as with the theory proposed by Sigmund Freud). Culture is how we define ourselves as individuals—different cultures promote different versions of individuality—and this is achieved not by defining an individual but by defining an “ideal” individual through a global acceptance of individualism. Individualism—an emphasis on personal aspects such as personal goals, personal uniqueness and personal control and in contrast marginalizing social aspects such as community, family and civics. The only way that individualism can grow is by developing these unique qualities for the self through abstract traits (Baumeiser, 1998). There are no examples in reality that reflect individualism—we have to create them ourselves through our construction of gods. It is this abstract nature of individualism through our construction of gods which is driving the narcissism epidemic. But these are not just abstract ideas, but ideas that have been embedded in our way of thinking.
There is no clear historical demarcation of when individualism gained a significant footing in our personality. The historians Jacob Burckhardt and Jules Michelet discuss how the growth of individualism can be seen around the dawn of the Renaissance period (Skidmore, 1996). And we can see how social context promotes individualism. But the first endorsement that individualism is a positive attribute was by Thomas Hobbes. Hobbes’s first law of nature states that man has the right to do whatever it takes to get what he wants, even if it means harming others. The only compromise is through Hobbe’s second law of nature, which states that in a consensus people can give up some rights (of their individualism) to live peacefully in a society, without conflict. Ayn Ryand takes this form of narcissism further with her radical and dysfunctional interpretation that individuals should not compromise. Individualism is enshrined as an ideal despite the harm to society and to the community. The individual trumps all other causes. Both individuality and the malformed extreme that we see through individualism are both social constructions. They are both an illusion since they exist relative to their social context. The argument against this self-centered growth towards individuality comes from a very unique place: Biology.
Against Individuality : Superorganisms
We talk about biological determinism as a negative philosophy whereby biology undermines any other influence especially in how we behave. But biologist themselves are eroding this biological imperative by conducting some amazing science. It was the sociologist Emil Durkheim who proposed that humans are “homo duplex”, leading double existences. According to Durkheim, one existence is rooted in biology and one in a social world. This interpretation holds amazing foresight at the time. This is an important distinction because while our social self (morally, intellectually, spiritually superior) is moving towards a more narcissistic form of individualism, the biology is moving in the opposite direction and showing how biologically diffuse we all are.
We are finding that the more we look at our body the more we see that we are made up of collective external organisms. Our bodies and our brain are not an exclusive entity—we have parts of other organisms and other people within us. In addition to genes that we inherit (in most cases, but not always) from both our parents, there are viruses, bacteria and potentially, other human cells within our body. Even our genes and brain are not deterministic and are influenced by external events.
Alien Cells in our Body
With 37 trillion cells in our body, Berg (1996) estimates that there are 10 times more bacterial cells in your body than human cells. Although bacteria is smaller and lighter than human cells—weighing 1-3% of our body weight—the 500-1,000 species of bacteria that inhabit our body have evolved with us for millions of years. Such mutual evolution is found in our mitochondria "the powerhouse of the cell" because they generate most of the cell's supply of chemical energy. In addition they are used for signaling, cellular differentiation, and cell death, as well as maintaining control of the cell cycle and cell growth. The presence of mitochondria in our cells varies with liver cells having more than 2000 mitochondria. Without mitochondria we will not survive since they are necessary to generate energy needed for the cell to function. It is humbling to learn that such an integrate part of our existence, these cells have their own genetic code and replicate independent of the rest of our cells. The reason for this is because mitochondria are a form of bacteria that were absorbed into our cells and now forms a symbiotic relationship with human cells—an endosymbiotic relationships in our body. However, in some cases the bacteria stay as independent contractors.
As independent contractors bacteria reside all over our body—inside and out—but bacteria has a special place in our human gut. Here in the dark recesses of our plumping reside trillions of microorganisms engaged in fermenting, killing off other harmful bacteria and viruses, enhancing our immune system and producing vitamins and hormones. This bacterial activity is so necessary to the body that their outcome function as an independent organ—a virtual "forgotten" organ. Gut bacteria help extract energy and nutrients from our food. This sharing of benefits shows in experiments where bacteria-free rodents have to consume nearly a third more calories than normal rodents to maintain their body weight. Such symbiotic relationship has direct implications for older adults.
In 2012 Marcus Claesson and Ian Jeffery from University College Cork in Ireland and their colleagues found that institutionalized older adults have a different bacteria in their gut than community older adults and younger people. And they related this change—caused by a restricted diet—to becoming weaker physically and increase mortality. That an alien microorganism can have such dramatic life enhancing properties is startling. But this revelation was overshadowed in December 2014 when Martin Blaser from New York University and Glenn Webb from Vanderbilt University, Nashville, Tennessee, tried to explain how bacteria directly kill older adults. They argue that modern medical problems, such as inflammation-induced early cancer, resistance to infectious diseases and degenerative diseases are in response to bacterial change, as we get older. Bacteria that live with us have learned to kill us off in old age. Using mathematical models the authors show that bacteria evolved because they contributed to the stability of early human populations: an evolutionary process that enhanced the survivability of younger adults while increasing vulnerability of older adults. In our modern world such bacteria's legacy is now a burden on human longevity. But bacteria is not just a passive guest. Sometimes bacteria can call for delivery.
Gut microbes can produce neurotransmitters that alter your mood and even may control your appetite. Causing you to crave food bacteria enjoys but which might be detrimental to your overall health. Such risky behaviors, in some cases, causes an earlier death. An infection of a parasite called Toxoplasma gondii, for example makes rats attracted to cats. Since the bacteria can reproduce only in cats (their vector) they make the rats lethargic around cats improving the chances of the rat being caught and improving the bacteria’s chances of infecting the cat and reproducing. In humans the same microbe increases the chance they will suffer from schizophrenia or suicidal depression.
Bacteria is not the only alien organism in our bodies. While we are being incubated, in the fetus, cells pass between twins or triplets and sometimes from previous siblings that occupied the womb. Around 8% of non-identical twins and 21% of triplets, for example, have not one, but two blood groups: one produced by their own cells, and one absorbed from their twin. There are even examples (anecdotal ABC News, 2014) where mothers passed on her twin sister’s genes, and not her own, to her children. Her eggs carried different genes from the rest of the body.
Alternatively, cells from an older sibling might stay around the mother’s body, only to find their way into your body after you are conceived. Lee Nelson from the University of Washington is examining whether cells from the mother herself may be implanted in the baby’s brain and the other way round where a baby’s genetic material finds itself in the mother’s brain. Nelson took slices of women’s brain tissue and screened their genome for signs of the Y-chromosome. Around 63% of mothers had Y-chromosome male cells in multiple brain regions. The authors cite a correlational observation that shows that these alien cells seemed to decrease the chances that the mother would subsequently develop Alzheimer’s--though exactly why remains a mystery.
Our body is home to a universe of external components. Not only is our body permeable to outside organisms, our brain is similarly influenced by external events, both in terms of how it functions and in terms of how it behaves.
Mirror Neurons
We have specialized areas in our brain that “mirror” our environment. In the 1980s, the Italian Giacomo Rizzolatti and his colleagues at the University of Parma, first observed mirror neurons in monkeys. Although mirror neurons exist in most animals, in humans they have been observed in multiple areas of the brain, with as many as 10 percent of neural cells devoted to mirroring. A mirror neuron fires both when a person acts and also while observing the same action performed by another person. Such mirror neurons respond directly to what is observed outside. Our brain responds and mimics the activation of another person’s behavior and activity. Oberman & Ramachandran (2009) believe that the existence of mirror neurons explain the development of self-awareness and reflection because humans can have “meta-representations of our own earlier brain processes” (Ramachandran, 2009). The individual is looking more diffuse and more dependent on its immediate environment. Even our genetic material is now more likely to be influenced by our environment that we had previously thought.
Epigenetics
Living in poor and dangerous neighborhoods has a direct effect on our hormones and stress chemicals—such as interleukin 6, acting as both a pro-inflammatory cytokine and an anti-inflammatory myokine that indicates body stress. A stressful environment—such as poor neighborhood—results in negative changes in the chemical composition of older adults, regardless of other factors. And these chemicals initiate changes in the body that are longer lasting because they switch on and off the expression of some genes. These epi-genes (above genes) can be switched on and off in order to help establishing and maintain a consistent optimum level of chemical balance within the body. Environmental factors such as mercury in water, second-hand smoke, diet including foliate, pharmaceuticals, pesticides, air pollutants, industrial chemicals, heavy metals, hormones in water, nutrition, and behavior have been shown to affect epi-genetics. Furthermore, epi-genetic changes are associated with specific outcomes such as cancer, diabetes, obesity, infertility, respiratory diseases, allergies, and neurodegenerative disorders such as Parkinson’s and Alzheimer’s diseases. Our body changes our epi-genes—establishing an optimum level of chemical balance in response to our environment that can then influence our overall health.
This accumulating evidence suggests that the body is a meeting place of interaction, a venue with the outside world—the geography, the community and significant others. Accepting that there is not just a "me" inside us but also a "we" then there is a more concise understanding how the environment, community, family and friends can determine our behavior and outcomes. My individuality is no longer solely about me but about my upbringing, my community and the people around me. Eroding the exclusivity of the individual exposes the extreme deception of elevating individualism as an ideal state.
Societal Implications
In reaction to the rise of individualism has been the concept of monasticism—living in a closed community with people that we share similar beliefs. Such an experiment was initially started by Epicurus and later evolved to the monastic life we see represented today in both religious communities such as among monks and nuns but also in social groups such as kibbutz, some “houses” in universities and the largest monastic living, prisons. Whereas society is moving towards a generation of younger adults believing that individualism will bring them happiness at the same time we are seeing groups of people being treated less than human. Hobbes’s first law of nature that man has the right to do whatever it takes to get what he wants, might not involve a compromise if only another group of people give up their rights. While the winning group are thinking like gods, another group are made to take responsibility for all the negative events that happen.
Hofstede (2001) observed that poorer countries were more likely to be collectivistic whereas wealthy countries were individualistic in nature. Dimensions of individualism and collectivism seem to be affected by economic factors such as wealth or poverty. Not only are there rich and poor countries/ individualist vs collectivism, but each society is becoming more divided. There are people behaving like gods and there people treated less than humans. This is what the Roman historian Sallust (Gaius Sallustius Crispus, 86–35bc) identified that: "We have public poverty and private opulence". We have yet again reached this time in history where a group of people is in poverty and a smaller group in private opulence, behaving and thinking that they are gods.
Emil Durkheim argued that there will be a conflict between the biological and the social aspect of the homo duplex but he could not have predicted that it was the biology that made us more collective. There might be a separation of homo duplex, where one group becomes more godlike and another falls from heaven. There must be a story there somewhere.
References
ABC NEWS (2014). She’s her own Twin. Accessed 10/12/2015
http://abcnews.go.com/Primetime/shes-twin/story?id=2315693
Baumeister, R. F. (1998). The self. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (4th ed., pp. 680-740). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Berg, R. (1996). "The indigenous gastrointestinal microflora". Trends in Microbiology 4 (11): 430–5. doi:10.1016/0966-842X(96)10057-3. PMID 8950812.,
Bell M.G. (2010). Agent Human: Consciousness At The Service Of The Group. Kindle edition.
Bianconi, E., Piovesan, A., Facchin, F., Beraudi, A., Casadei, R., Frabetti, F., ... & Canaider, S. (2013). An estimation of the number of cells in the human body. Annals of human biology, 40(6), 463-471.
Hofstede, G. (2001). Culture’s consequences: Comparing values, behaviors, institutions and organizations across nations (2nd ed.). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Lerner, M.J. (1980). The belief in a just world: A fundamental delusion. New York: Plenum Press.
Oberman, L. & Ramachandran, V.S. (2009). "Reflections on the Mirror Neuron System: Their Evolutionary Functions Beyond Motor Representation". In Pineda, J.A. Mirror Neuron Systems: The Role of Mirroring Processes in Social Cognition. Humana Press. pp. 39–62.
Ramachandran, V.S. (2009). "Self Awareness: The Last Frontier, Edge Foundation web essay". Retrieved July 26, 2011.Skidmore, M. J. (1996). Renaissance to millennium: Ideological insights from creative works. The European Legacy, 1(4), 1628-1633.
Triandis, H. C., & Gelfand, M. J. (2012). A theory of individualism and collectivism. Handbook of theories of social psychology, 2, 498-520.
Turner, J. C. (1982). Towards a cognitive redefinition of the social group. In H. Tajfel (ed.), Social Identity and Intergroup Relations. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Twenge, J. M., & Campbell, W. K. (2009). The narcissism epidemic: Living in the age of entitlement. Simon and Schuster.
© USA Copyrighted 2014 Mario D. Garrett
In our mind we have a model of the world. The sole purpose of having such a complex brain is to represent the world in its entirety as far as it affects us. Every day we adjust this model to make it closer to reality. An unachievable objective since reality is ephemeral but we make reality conform to patterns that help us to predict it. Both our dreams and our waking emotions signal a need to modify and adjust this view of the world. And this cognitive representation remains mostly unconscious. Our brain interacts unconsciously first and only gives us consciousness when it requires our full attention in order to address some complex event. This is the internal world that is driving us to think like gods. This unconscious model of the world furnishes us with a feeling of mastery and control because we can predict and affect change. But this feeling of mastery is an illusion and it is this illusion that is growing.
Rousseau's ‘Malo periculosam libertatem quam quietum servitium” If gods were people, they would govern themselves democratically. In our cognitive model the world is predictable and just (Lerner, 1980). Despite an onslaught of daily news informing us otherwise, we still believe in a just world. We continue to be surprised by disasters or catastrophes thinking that they are exceptions. They are not. They are exceptions only in our model of the world—the one that we cultivate in our head, the virtual box—because everything is in harmony, everything is balanced, and just. We continue to aspire to a world where we can "cure" death, "regain" our youth, "fight" terrorism, "save" humanity...these are illogical and delusional aims only if you are NOT a god. If we aspire to behave like—or think that we are—gods, then these aspirations are attainable. These aspirations confer a delusional sense of control over our world.
The Emergence of Individuality
The idea that we are somewhat godlike requires that we have a belief that we are unique individuals. Jean Twenge and Keith Campbell exploration of the narcissistic epidemic (Twenge & Campbell, 2009) document an alarming rise of narcissism at every level of our society. With the pampering of social media promoting a world filled with egos, our individualistic selves are thriving. We only have to look at the world economy where such individualistic arrogance is promoted by banks, despite resulting in risky, unrealistic investments. A financial system that enshrines the motto that it is "too big to fail". Godlike structures which are resilient to reality and to change, that rest above the law and it seems above basic economics. And such arrogance comes across from individuals running these structures. Among the oligarch—the very rich—there has always been godlike arrogance. Traditionally oligarch were philanthropic, they wanted to change the world for the better. But it is their view of “better” that is creating inequity. Not only are we turning into a world—not just a society—run by oligarchs, but we are seeing these inequities transferred across generations. The oligarchs have discovered immortality they are transferring their wealth down successive generations. Their individualism will live into eternity though the management of their wealth. These are not delusions of grandeur, since these individuals have the power to affect great social changes. And oligarchs have always been with us, although perhaps not to such as extent as today. What is uniquely transformative is the emerging belief that anyone can have this power. This form of individualism has infected the general public. And it is this belief—that anyone can become an oligarch—allows us to sell short our collective futures.
Individualism should not be confused with individuality. Individuality is a psychological concept of self that is separate and distinct from others. Individualism is a historical interpretation of self as the center of all interest and a belief that individual achievement rather than community or societal progress is the ultimate development. That the moral and intellectual imperative resides with the individual and not the community, something that is now shared with corporations (cf. Lukes 1990). Individualism is a deformity of individuality. There is a certain form, a shape that individuality takes that fits in within society. But through individualism, this shape become malformed, and no longer fits within the bigger puzzle of society. It stands out as separate and, in some cases, an antithesis to the social setting it resides in. Individualism is predicated on the expectations that wellbeing and life satisfaction are achieved only through one’s personal goals rather than through community achievements (Diener and Diener 1995). An offshoot of this is that we base our predictions—judgment, reasoning and causal inference—based on the person (or people) rather than the situation or social context (Morris and Peng, 1994). We judge others, and not the context that we find them in. Terrorists are deranged people rather than sane people in deranged contexts. Our godlike behavior elevates control of behavior to internal thought rather than social contexts.
There is evidence that in pre-history—before written records—humans were aware of their individuality but without individualism. Historically our personality was shared with the community that we lived in. Bell provides numerous examples where “there is wide agreement among anthropologists, evolutionists and cognitive specialists that early humans had little or no awareness of themselves as independent personalities, but instead felt themselves to be parts of the group (collective) to which they belonged.” (Bell, 2010) Remnants of this common sharing is still seen in marriage rituals were the marriage is seen as uniting two families together, in as much as a coupling of two individuals.
The growth of individualism has been identified in psychiatry through the work on personality. Personality is a hypothetical entity that cannot be observed or studied other than when studied within interpersonal situations. There is no “I” in personality unless there is an interaction with others. The “I” without a social interpersonal context does not exist. In Social Identity Theory 'individuals define themselves in terms of their social group memberships and that group-defined self-perception produces psychologically distinctive effects in social behaviour' (Turner, 1982). This socialization is what makes us distinct. As early as the 1950s, Harry Sullivan in “The Illusion of Individuality” argued that: “…human beings are human animals that have been filled with culture—socialized…” (p. 323) His personality theory is based on relationships rather than internal psychodynamics (such as with the theory proposed by Sigmund Freud). Culture is how we define ourselves as individuals—different cultures promote different versions of individuality—and this is achieved not by defining an individual but by defining an “ideal” individual through a global acceptance of individualism. Individualism—an emphasis on personal aspects such as personal goals, personal uniqueness and personal control and in contrast marginalizing social aspects such as community, family and civics. The only way that individualism can grow is by developing these unique qualities for the self through abstract traits (Baumeiser, 1998). There are no examples in reality that reflect individualism—we have to create them ourselves through our construction of gods. It is this abstract nature of individualism through our construction of gods which is driving the narcissism epidemic. But these are not just abstract ideas, but ideas that have been embedded in our way of thinking.
There is no clear historical demarcation of when individualism gained a significant footing in our personality. The historians Jacob Burckhardt and Jules Michelet discuss how the growth of individualism can be seen around the dawn of the Renaissance period (Skidmore, 1996). And we can see how social context promotes individualism. But the first endorsement that individualism is a positive attribute was by Thomas Hobbes. Hobbes’s first law of nature states that man has the right to do whatever it takes to get what he wants, even if it means harming others. The only compromise is through Hobbe’s second law of nature, which states that in a consensus people can give up some rights (of their individualism) to live peacefully in a society, without conflict. Ayn Ryand takes this form of narcissism further with her radical and dysfunctional interpretation that individuals should not compromise. Individualism is enshrined as an ideal despite the harm to society and to the community. The individual trumps all other causes. Both individuality and the malformed extreme that we see through individualism are both social constructions. They are both an illusion since they exist relative to their social context. The argument against this self-centered growth towards individuality comes from a very unique place: Biology.
Against Individuality : Superorganisms
We talk about biological determinism as a negative philosophy whereby biology undermines any other influence especially in how we behave. But biologist themselves are eroding this biological imperative by conducting some amazing science. It was the sociologist Emil Durkheim who proposed that humans are “homo duplex”, leading double existences. According to Durkheim, one existence is rooted in biology and one in a social world. This interpretation holds amazing foresight at the time. This is an important distinction because while our social self (morally, intellectually, spiritually superior) is moving towards a more narcissistic form of individualism, the biology is moving in the opposite direction and showing how biologically diffuse we all are.
We are finding that the more we look at our body the more we see that we are made up of collective external organisms. Our bodies and our brain are not an exclusive entity—we have parts of other organisms and other people within us. In addition to genes that we inherit (in most cases, but not always) from both our parents, there are viruses, bacteria and potentially, other human cells within our body. Even our genes and brain are not deterministic and are influenced by external events.
Alien Cells in our Body
With 37 trillion cells in our body, Berg (1996) estimates that there are 10 times more bacterial cells in your body than human cells. Although bacteria is smaller and lighter than human cells—weighing 1-3% of our body weight—the 500-1,000 species of bacteria that inhabit our body have evolved with us for millions of years. Such mutual evolution is found in our mitochondria "the powerhouse of the cell" because they generate most of the cell's supply of chemical energy. In addition they are used for signaling, cellular differentiation, and cell death, as well as maintaining control of the cell cycle and cell growth. The presence of mitochondria in our cells varies with liver cells having more than 2000 mitochondria. Without mitochondria we will not survive since they are necessary to generate energy needed for the cell to function. It is humbling to learn that such an integrate part of our existence, these cells have their own genetic code and replicate independent of the rest of our cells. The reason for this is because mitochondria are a form of bacteria that were absorbed into our cells and now forms a symbiotic relationship with human cells—an endosymbiotic relationships in our body. However, in some cases the bacteria stay as independent contractors.
As independent contractors bacteria reside all over our body—inside and out—but bacteria has a special place in our human gut. Here in the dark recesses of our plumping reside trillions of microorganisms engaged in fermenting, killing off other harmful bacteria and viruses, enhancing our immune system and producing vitamins and hormones. This bacterial activity is so necessary to the body that their outcome function as an independent organ—a virtual "forgotten" organ. Gut bacteria help extract energy and nutrients from our food. This sharing of benefits shows in experiments where bacteria-free rodents have to consume nearly a third more calories than normal rodents to maintain their body weight. Such symbiotic relationship has direct implications for older adults.
In 2012 Marcus Claesson and Ian Jeffery from University College Cork in Ireland and their colleagues found that institutionalized older adults have a different bacteria in their gut than community older adults and younger people. And they related this change—caused by a restricted diet—to becoming weaker physically and increase mortality. That an alien microorganism can have such dramatic life enhancing properties is startling. But this revelation was overshadowed in December 2014 when Martin Blaser from New York University and Glenn Webb from Vanderbilt University, Nashville, Tennessee, tried to explain how bacteria directly kill older adults. They argue that modern medical problems, such as inflammation-induced early cancer, resistance to infectious diseases and degenerative diseases are in response to bacterial change, as we get older. Bacteria that live with us have learned to kill us off in old age. Using mathematical models the authors show that bacteria evolved because they contributed to the stability of early human populations: an evolutionary process that enhanced the survivability of younger adults while increasing vulnerability of older adults. In our modern world such bacteria's legacy is now a burden on human longevity. But bacteria is not just a passive guest. Sometimes bacteria can call for delivery.
Gut microbes can produce neurotransmitters that alter your mood and even may control your appetite. Causing you to crave food bacteria enjoys but which might be detrimental to your overall health. Such risky behaviors, in some cases, causes an earlier death. An infection of a parasite called Toxoplasma gondii, for example makes rats attracted to cats. Since the bacteria can reproduce only in cats (their vector) they make the rats lethargic around cats improving the chances of the rat being caught and improving the bacteria’s chances of infecting the cat and reproducing. In humans the same microbe increases the chance they will suffer from schizophrenia or suicidal depression.
Bacteria is not the only alien organism in our bodies. While we are being incubated, in the fetus, cells pass between twins or triplets and sometimes from previous siblings that occupied the womb. Around 8% of non-identical twins and 21% of triplets, for example, have not one, but two blood groups: one produced by their own cells, and one absorbed from their twin. There are even examples (anecdotal ABC News, 2014) where mothers passed on her twin sister’s genes, and not her own, to her children. Her eggs carried different genes from the rest of the body.
Alternatively, cells from an older sibling might stay around the mother’s body, only to find their way into your body after you are conceived. Lee Nelson from the University of Washington is examining whether cells from the mother herself may be implanted in the baby’s brain and the other way round where a baby’s genetic material finds itself in the mother’s brain. Nelson took slices of women’s brain tissue and screened their genome for signs of the Y-chromosome. Around 63% of mothers had Y-chromosome male cells in multiple brain regions. The authors cite a correlational observation that shows that these alien cells seemed to decrease the chances that the mother would subsequently develop Alzheimer’s--though exactly why remains a mystery.
Our body is home to a universe of external components. Not only is our body permeable to outside organisms, our brain is similarly influenced by external events, both in terms of how it functions and in terms of how it behaves.
Mirror Neurons
We have specialized areas in our brain that “mirror” our environment. In the 1980s, the Italian Giacomo Rizzolatti and his colleagues at the University of Parma, first observed mirror neurons in monkeys. Although mirror neurons exist in most animals, in humans they have been observed in multiple areas of the brain, with as many as 10 percent of neural cells devoted to mirroring. A mirror neuron fires both when a person acts and also while observing the same action performed by another person. Such mirror neurons respond directly to what is observed outside. Our brain responds and mimics the activation of another person’s behavior and activity. Oberman & Ramachandran (2009) believe that the existence of mirror neurons explain the development of self-awareness and reflection because humans can have “meta-representations of our own earlier brain processes” (Ramachandran, 2009). The individual is looking more diffuse and more dependent on its immediate environment. Even our genetic material is now more likely to be influenced by our environment that we had previously thought.
Epigenetics
Living in poor and dangerous neighborhoods has a direct effect on our hormones and stress chemicals—such as interleukin 6, acting as both a pro-inflammatory cytokine and an anti-inflammatory myokine that indicates body stress. A stressful environment—such as poor neighborhood—results in negative changes in the chemical composition of older adults, regardless of other factors. And these chemicals initiate changes in the body that are longer lasting because they switch on and off the expression of some genes. These epi-genes (above genes) can be switched on and off in order to help establishing and maintain a consistent optimum level of chemical balance within the body. Environmental factors such as mercury in water, second-hand smoke, diet including foliate, pharmaceuticals, pesticides, air pollutants, industrial chemicals, heavy metals, hormones in water, nutrition, and behavior have been shown to affect epi-genetics. Furthermore, epi-genetic changes are associated with specific outcomes such as cancer, diabetes, obesity, infertility, respiratory diseases, allergies, and neurodegenerative disorders such as Parkinson’s and Alzheimer’s diseases. Our body changes our epi-genes—establishing an optimum level of chemical balance in response to our environment that can then influence our overall health.
This accumulating evidence suggests that the body is a meeting place of interaction, a venue with the outside world—the geography, the community and significant others. Accepting that there is not just a "me" inside us but also a "we" then there is a more concise understanding how the environment, community, family and friends can determine our behavior and outcomes. My individuality is no longer solely about me but about my upbringing, my community and the people around me. Eroding the exclusivity of the individual exposes the extreme deception of elevating individualism as an ideal state.
Societal Implications
In reaction to the rise of individualism has been the concept of monasticism—living in a closed community with people that we share similar beliefs. Such an experiment was initially started by Epicurus and later evolved to the monastic life we see represented today in both religious communities such as among monks and nuns but also in social groups such as kibbutz, some “houses” in universities and the largest monastic living, prisons. Whereas society is moving towards a generation of younger adults believing that individualism will bring them happiness at the same time we are seeing groups of people being treated less than human. Hobbes’s first law of nature that man has the right to do whatever it takes to get what he wants, might not involve a compromise if only another group of people give up their rights. While the winning group are thinking like gods, another group are made to take responsibility for all the negative events that happen.
Hofstede (2001) observed that poorer countries were more likely to be collectivistic whereas wealthy countries were individualistic in nature. Dimensions of individualism and collectivism seem to be affected by economic factors such as wealth or poverty. Not only are there rich and poor countries/ individualist vs collectivism, but each society is becoming more divided. There are people behaving like gods and there people treated less than humans. This is what the Roman historian Sallust (Gaius Sallustius Crispus, 86–35bc) identified that: "We have public poverty and private opulence". We have yet again reached this time in history where a group of people is in poverty and a smaller group in private opulence, behaving and thinking that they are gods.
Emil Durkheim argued that there will be a conflict between the biological and the social aspect of the homo duplex but he could not have predicted that it was the biology that made us more collective. There might be a separation of homo duplex, where one group becomes more godlike and another falls from heaven. There must be a story there somewhere.
References
ABC NEWS (2014). She’s her own Twin. Accessed 10/12/2015
http://abcnews.go.com/Primetime/shes-twin/story?id=2315693
Baumeister, R. F. (1998). The self. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (4th ed., pp. 680-740). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Berg, R. (1996). "The indigenous gastrointestinal microflora". Trends in Microbiology 4 (11): 430–5. doi:10.1016/0966-842X(96)10057-3. PMID 8950812.,
Bell M.G. (2010). Agent Human: Consciousness At The Service Of The Group. Kindle edition.
Bianconi, E., Piovesan, A., Facchin, F., Beraudi, A., Casadei, R., Frabetti, F., ... & Canaider, S. (2013). An estimation of the number of cells in the human body. Annals of human biology, 40(6), 463-471.
Hofstede, G. (2001). Culture’s consequences: Comparing values, behaviors, institutions and organizations across nations (2nd ed.). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Lerner, M.J. (1980). The belief in a just world: A fundamental delusion. New York: Plenum Press.
Oberman, L. & Ramachandran, V.S. (2009). "Reflections on the Mirror Neuron System: Their Evolutionary Functions Beyond Motor Representation". In Pineda, J.A. Mirror Neuron Systems: The Role of Mirroring Processes in Social Cognition. Humana Press. pp. 39–62.
Ramachandran, V.S. (2009). "Self Awareness: The Last Frontier, Edge Foundation web essay". Retrieved July 26, 2011.Skidmore, M. J. (1996). Renaissance to millennium: Ideological insights from creative works. The European Legacy, 1(4), 1628-1633.
Triandis, H. C., & Gelfand, M. J. (2012). A theory of individualism and collectivism. Handbook of theories of social psychology, 2, 498-520.
Turner, J. C. (1982). Towards a cognitive redefinition of the social group. In H. Tajfel (ed.), Social Identity and Intergroup Relations. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Twenge, J. M., & Campbell, W. K. (2009). The narcissism epidemic: Living in the age of entitlement. Simon and Schuster.
© USA Copyrighted 2014 Mario D. Garrett
Saturday, October 31, 2015
Why Death is Important
It seems that humans cannot come to grips with death. Even
when someone has died, we hold on to shreds of belief about their continued
existence in realms that are independent of us. This vestige of residual
existence is represented throughout all religions, to varying degrees of
realism and ceremony. Our present clinical age has transformed death from a
natural—but incomprehensible—cycle of life to one of clinical failure. Death is
a medical embarrassment.
Of all disciplines, biologists are perhaps at an advantage
in accepting death not only as a natural process but as a necessary process.
Leonard Hayflick, the renowned biologist/gerontologist was perhaps the most
succinct in saying that (paraphrasing) “death might be detrimental to the
individual but necessary for the species.” Biologists understand death because
they look at species and how species develop. Because higher turnover (death
rate) means that the species is more adaptive—these are known as r-selection
coined by the ecologists Robert MacArthur and E. O. Wilson (Pianka, 1970). The
alternative biological strategy would be to have fewer offspring but to invest more
into their nurturing (such as humans.) This type of strategy is referred to as K-selection
species. Biologists are so good at dealing with death that they categorized
species on the basis of their death rate.
Such an important construct as death should have more
relevance to us as humans. And it does, especially when we need to understand
the foundation for our sense of being, as in metaphysics--a branch of
philosophy interested in the first principle of things. Metaphysics asks
radical questions include abstract concepts such as being, knowing, substance,
cause, identity, time, and space. How can we understand that we are not just
performing actors on the stage of life following a genetic narrative, but that
we are participating directors as well. It seems that death—an idea of the
expectation of death—provides us with an urgency to live. When Simon Critchley
compiled the thoughts about death by more than 190 philosophers, the central
theme that he summarized was the idea that death provides an urgency to live in
the present. Philosophers use the concept of death to define the interactions at
the present as the only real aspect of the passage of time. The idea of death
defines our idea of the reality of the present. But death has to be more than
idea. At the turn of the 1900s Sigmund Freud was the first one to assign the
idea of death as a drive.
Thanatos--the hypothesis of a death drive, that lead to an
inanimate state--was originally proposed by Sigmund Freud in 1920 in Beyond the
Pleasure Principle. Freud was trying to explain the First World War. How can
virile men willingly go to their death rather than follow their true desire for
sexual gratification? However Freud’s interpretation of why patients repeated
relive the traumatic experience as it is still happening to them now (as
personal), rather than as a past and abstract experience (as actors) indicates
a certain lack of understand of the ontological belief of how the self, the “I”
remains constant across time. For such an interpretation Martin Heidegger has a
better interpretation of death.
Martin Heidegger’s book “Being and Time” refers to time as
finite defined at the end by our understanding of death. In our being, death
provides the final full stop/period. To be an authentic human being, we must be
aware of our ultimate death. This is what Heidegger famously calls
"being-towards-death". Heidegger needed death in order for us to
care. For Heidegger, caring is not being nurturing and showing empathy, for
Heidegger caring is owning your being. To care we have to appreciate death and
because we cannot truly know and experience death we have to accept the "possibility
of impossibility”--our non-existence. One cannot fully live unless one
confronts one's own mortality through a courageous "anxiety" (Heidegger,
1927, p. 310). Michel de Montaigne said this much better when he said that: "The
premeditation of death is the premeditation of liberty; he who has learned to
die has unlearned to serve.” (Montaigne, 2012, Chapter XIX). This anxiety about dying is why we care--we feel
responsible for our lives. It is the primary fulcrum that energizes human
engagement in a world that we own, that is personal and not a backdrop for a
theatrical existence.
Death is important in constructing theories about how people
behave because death--and our internal appreciation of death—means that we
start to care about our world, our behavior and existence. All philosophers have discussed death, some in
passing other in more detail. However Heidegger’s interpretation of pinning the
basis of knowing about oneself on the idea that we have an appreciation of our
ultimate non-existence is the strongest. Freud’ analysis is too specific to a
wish to die, which does not translate well nowadays with our narcissistic
cohorts. Heidegger’s interpretation does however suggest that there is a
developmental process in that our appreciation of our own demise translates
directly to our caring to us owning our world and doing something about it.
References
Critchley S (2009). The book of dead philosophers. Vintage
Books.
Freud S (1920) Beyond the Pleasure Principle
Heidegger M (1927) Being and Time. Reprint, New York: Harper
and Row, 1962.
Montaigne deM (2012) The Essays of Montaigne. Reprinted Ebook.
Accessed on 10/31/2015 from: http://www.gutenberg.org/files/3600/3600-h/3600-h.htm
Pianka ER (1970). On r and K selection. American Naturalist
104 (940): 592–597.
© USA Copyrighted 2015 Mario D. Garrett
Wednesday, September 30, 2015
God, Mathematics and Psychology: Are they all one?
This discussion focuses on psychology and the philosophy of
mathematics and will contribute nothing to mathematical thought. Its aim is to
introduce mathematics as a creation of psychology. Sophisticated, complex and
ever evolving, but nevertheless psychology.
Mathematics
translates patterns into reducible parts. These parts form theorems—incremental
reasoning based on a chain of formal proofs—that conform to logic but operate
beyond logic. Mathematicians argue that these patterns are universal and real
and that the interconnecting system of reducible parts is what constitutes
mathematics—a language of spatial positioning, geometry, numbers, volume,
movement and patterns. These are complex patterns that lead to complex
theorems.
Sometimes
these patterns exist in reality and prove useful in terms of predicting
physical events in the universe and sometimes they are the perfect embodiment
of a cognitive world--true forms that exist primarily in our imagination, such
as the perfect circle. Sometimes the theorems relate to patterns that are
solely--as far as we know, or yet--in the realm of a group of mathematicians’
imagination. Although mathematics is not set-up, by mathematicians, to explain
our reality, there is however a symbiotic relationship, in that proofs can come
from within the physical experimental world.
The
basis for elevating mathematics to more than just a complex system of creating
theorems is the role that mathematics was given by Pythagoras (6th Century
BC). Pythagoras believed that numbers
were not only the way to truth, but truth itself. That mathematics not only
described the work of god, but was the way that god worked. This belief, that
mathematics holds an intrinsic truth remains with mathematicians today. They
believe that mathematics is the language of the gods. And that is a problem if
you do not believe in god or in an over-ridding principle of existence--none
that we can understand anyway. Science is by definition both atheist and
agnostic despite what individual scientists believe. Most mathematicians behave
as deist who believe that God created the universe but that natural laws
determine how the universe plays out. This is a Epicurean (341–270 BC) belief
that the gods are too busy to deal with the day-to-day running of the universe
but they set it in motion using mathematics.
Mathematicians
therefore argue that mathematics is a higher order that is found in reality.
But there are no examples of such proofs. Mathematicians argue that they are
more discoverers rather than inventors. But this dichotomy also seems false.
Mathematicians seem to do both, most often at the same time. The British philosopher Michael Dummett
suggests that mathematical theorems are prodded into existence--he uses the
term probing (Dummett, 1964). Using the analogy of the game of chess where, “It
is commonly supposed … that the game of chess is an abstract entity” (Dummett,
1973). But there is certainly a sense in which the game would not have existed
were it not for the mental activity of human beings. It is a delusion to
believe that just because we find a pleasing pattern, a game that resonates
across cultures, that the reason it is pleasing is because there is a god
behind it. But mathematicians argue that chess, or theorems are not entirely
products of our minds since there must already be something there to prod. But
the obverse argument is equally true that mathematical “truths” are entirely
dependent on us since we need to prod them to bring them into existence.
The
same is true for language, art, music and other “Third World” constructs—these
are incrementally evolving systems and form one of Karl Popper’s ontological
tools (Carr, 1977). Third World is where the system that is developed exists beyond
the creator. Language is an excellent example, although Third World also
includes abstract objects such as scientific theories, stories, myths, tools,
social institutions, and works of art. Language is incremental and ever
evolving, and is used to help us communicate reality. Within this Third World,
language as with mathematics, is also argued to be both discovered or invented.
Theory
of language development has oscillated between two schools of thought. One school that argues that language is
culture-bound, known as Descriptivists. And on the other side is the argument
that promotes language as part of our biological makeup, known as the
Generativists. As a Generativist, Chomsky (1980: p134) phrased it eloquently
when he said that, “we do not really learn language; rather, grammar grows in
the mind”. The analogy between formal
mathematical systems and human languages is not a new or novel idea. In fact
such formal language theory have already been established in its modern form by
Noam Chomsky in an attempt to systematically investigate the computational
basis of not just human language but has become applicable to a variety of
rule-governed system across multiple domains--computer programs, music, visual
patterns, animal vocalizations, RNA structure and even dance (Fitch &
Friederici, 2012). This symbiotic relationship exists across all Third World
constructs: mathematics and music, music and art, art and language and all
other permutations. As with mathematics, we refine language with time. Future generations
build upon language and mathematics and the only constraint seems to be our
psychology. Mathematics similarly has
this incremental nature. The last sentence of a talk given by Fine on
mathematics “The only constraint is our
imagination and what we find appropriate or pleasing.” (Fine, 2012: p27). What we find appropriate and pleasing is
where the psychology comes in and our clue to the inception of mathematics and
the description of our psychology.
As
a guide, we have to go back to earlier (and more simple) mathematics to
understand this principle of “pleasing.” Pythagoras and music is the basis for
a convergence between mathematics and psychology. Pythagoras (6th century BC) observed that
when the blacksmith struck his anvil, different notes were produced according
to the weight of the hammer. He later discovered that the ratio of the length
of two strings determines the octave "that the chief musical intervals are
expressible in simple mathematical ratios between the first four integers"
(Kirk & Raven, 1964: p.229). Thus, the "Octave=2:1, fifth=3:2,
fourth=4:3" (p.230). These ratios harmonize, meaning that are pleasing
both to the mind and to the ear. Although this mathematical system breaks down
the higher we go up the scale, there was a solution by adjusting the ratio of
the fifth so that it is commensurable with seven octaves. Seven octaves is
128:1, or 27. John Stillwell (2006) argues that "equal semitones" or
"equal temperament" (p.21), was developed almost simultaneously in
China and the Netherlnds, by Zhu Zaiyu (Chu Tsai-yü) in 1584 (during the Ming
Dynasty and by the Simon Steven in 1585 and by (Ross, 2001). But the point is
that a mathematical rule was developed on the basis of a harmony that we humans
find pleasing.
In
nature, all sounds are the same. The creator of the universe created all
acoustics, all sounds are perfect. Nature cannot discriminate among them since
they are all necessary and useful. As such, selecting harmonics is
psychological rather than godlike. We like the separation of scales because we
can psychologically compartmentalize the sound. We are creatures of order and
consistency and prefer to have distinct and distinguishable sounds. In reality
there is no such thing as harmonics, we look for it as humans because it is
pleasing.
Such
psychological preferences are automatic and require no processing and thinking
on our part. This automation can be easily be disrupted by playing a tone that
is ostensibly ever increasing or decreasing without end. Such a tone was
developed by Roger Shepard and consists of a superposition of sine waves
separated by octaves. This creates the auditory illusion of a tone that
continually ascends or descends in pitch, yet remaining constant.
Not
only does the Shepard Tone create dissonance because we find it difficult to
understand, it also creates uneasiness as a result of this dissonance. This
perceived auditory dissonance causes emotional uneasiness. We become uncomfortable when we cannot pigeon
hole our perception. We need sounds that are at a prescribed distance from each
other that make perception easier. Pythagoras defined the first mathematical
rule for auditory perception, the definition of an octave that pleases our
psychology for order and form. The fact that both European and Chinese figured
this out at the same time indicates that the perception of octave generalizes
across linguistic and auditory differences (for more auditory illusions see
Deutsch, 2011). These psychological requirements, codified into mathematics are
also found true for vision.
We
like to see things in “chunks.” Mathematics was the earliest discipline to
reflect this psychological need by inventing the number “one.” This basis of an
“entity” forms the upside down pyramid of mathematics. Without a “one” there is
no mathematics. But there are problems with the number one. There is a point at
which a “one” cannot be defined mathematically, or where it fails to conform to
some particular way, such as differentiability. This singularity--which is
proving to be so problematic for mathematicians in explaining quantum physics
for example--is only a problem for mathematicians, because an entity of “one”
is the perfect creation of our mind and not nature. In fact the only way that
quantum physics can explain superposition, entanglement and other quantum
mechanics is by removing the “one” from the theorem. By removing the
parenthesis around “one” quantum physics can be better explained, although then
we have to readdress our psychology and the reliance on our perception of separate
entities. From a psychological point this can be easier achieved rather than
forcing quantum physics to conform to psychology.
History
has been here before. Pythagoras--having traced the hand of god in how music is
constructed--thought that each of the seven planets produced particular notes
depending on its orbit around the earth. This was Musica Mundana and for
Pythagorians, different musical modes have different effects on the person who
hears them. Taking this a step further, the mathematician Boethius (480-524 AD)
explained that the soul and the body are subject to the same laws of proportion
that govern music and the cosmos itself. As the Italian semiotician Umberto Eco
observed we are happiest when we conform to these laws because "we love
similarity, but hate and resent dissimilarity" (Eco, 2002; p31).
This
is not the first time that mathematicians thought they have touched the hand of
god, neither will it be the last time. But what Pythagoras touched is our
psychology. By focusing on pleasing patterns, similarities, and order,
mathematicians are exploring the foundations of our psyche. And to do this they
had to build rules and “common notions” that bind all these thoughts into a
coherent language that translates into mathematics. For example if we take
Euclid (4th Century BC) five "common notions” as defined in The Elements:
• Things that are equal to the same thing are
also equal to one another
• If equals are added to equals, then the wholes
are equal
• If equals are
subtracted from equals, then the remainders are equal
• Things that
coincide with one another are equal to one another
• The whole is
greater than the part.
There is an unambiguous relationship with
classic Euclidian mathematics and Gestalt psychology. Gestalt psychology has rules
that mirror these Euclidian common notions (Lagopoulos
& Boklund-Lagopoulou, 1992).
But there have been further developments. The prolific Swiss psychologist Jean
Piaget (1896–1980) while investigating children’s conception of space
discovered highly abstract mathematical structures in the child’s primordial
conception of space. He argues that the
further development of geometric space should not be understood as reflecting
the capacity of the child’s developing physiological functions, but as a product
of the child’s interaction with the world. The child constantly builds up specific
structures of perception and reorganizes spatial conception. Accordingly, Euclid’s
elements and the topological properties of shapes have their origin neither in
the world nor in the history of sciences, but in cognitive schemes that we
build up in our daily interaction with objects.
The same understanding—that there are mathematical
structure embedded in our cognitive processes—precludes the need for either
mathematical or language. These theorems exist independent because that is how
the brain is structured. A good example of this pre-mathematics and
pre-linguistic ability is provided by a tribe that does not have a concept of
numbers in its language. Dan Everett’s description of the Pirahã language of
the southern Amazon basin exposes the tangled relationship between mathematical
constructs and our cognitive capacity (Everett 2012). The Pirahã language has
no clause subordination (e.g. after, because, if) at all, indeed it has no
grammatical embedding of any kind, and it has no quantifier words (e.g. many,
few, none); and it has no number words at all (e.g. one, two, many). But they can still count and perform quit
complex mathematical comparisons despite the lack of linguistic structure. The
main deficit is that they cannot memorize these functions. So they can perform
mathematical functions only for the immediate situation. In Popperian terms,
they do not have a Third World construct of mathematics to enable them to retain
an abstract representation of numbers which mathematicians, through their use
of mathematical language can. And mathematicians have created this language,
this mathematics where “one” forms the foundation.
Mathematics
however has evolved and built upon this concept of “one.” It would be naïve to
assume that mathematics has stood still as a discipline. Although the early
conception of “one” is very restrictive number, in which ‘number’ means
‘natural number’ mathematics evolved to adopt a less restrictive conception of
“one” in which it means ‘integer’; then meaning rationals; then reals, and then
complex numbers. With such creations, there is a more nuanced appreciation of the
finite interpretations of “one”. In psychology we might distinguish a human being
(aka one), and then talk about aggregate or composite features such as family,
community, or head, eyes, nose (reals), and then complex numbers such becoming
a millionaire, getting divorced, losing a limb, becoming blind (complex
numbers.) Mathematics has not extended the domain of numbers, but liberalized what
we mean by ‘number’ and as a collinearity what we mean by “one.” Our
presumption that there is a single number “one” and that, in extending the
number system we simply add and perform “functions” to the numbers that were
already there is not what mathematics has become. There are as many number “ones”
as there are types of numbers. But by redefining the meaning we are creating a
new definition of “one”. One that is less suspect to investigation and study,
and bears less of a relationship with anything tangible (Fine, 2012).
We
think in very complex ways that is still not understood, continues to be
misrepresented and remains misunderstood. The human brain has more synaptic
transmissions than we have stars in the universe. The capacity for human
thought is immense. Clues are emerging that we think in very abstract ways that
mirror the development of theorems in mathematics. Holographic theory of
thinking is just one crude method of representing this universe of thought. It
is plausible that mathematics could be a portal to understanding our psyche,
our art and our behavior. We could learn our limitations, and our attributes
and allow for the exploration of a process that we do not yet know and cannot
know. We grow up developing our thinking as theorems--despite that in some
cases our language does not accommodate such thinking--we still use innate
mathematics to develop our sense of numbers and patterns. Mathematics is our
way of thinking. We simply grow out of
it, as do mathematicians who simply grow out of being brilliant mathematicians
and converge into cultural thought (language, roles and cultural morals.)
Mathematicians have a short life of brilliance since their natural thought
processes are eventually taken over by pragmatic concerns. Such is the final
objective of our brain, survival in the real experiential world. Survival in a
sentient world—a world dominated by feeling and experiencing. But mathematics
can form the basis of formalizing theories of our thinking processes, mind
sensations and feelings. We need to see
beyond the silos of disciplines and view our humanity as more than pitting
humans against the hand of god, and simply see the hand of god as our own
genius waiting to be acknowledged.
References
Carr B (1977). Popper's Third World. The
Philosophical Quarterly Vol. 27, No. 108, pp. 214-226
Diana Deutsch Accessed 8/20/2015::
http://deutsch.ucsd.edu/psychology/pages.php?i=201)
Dummett M (1964) Bringing about the
past. Philosophical Review 73: 338–59.
Eco U (2002). Art and beauty in the
middle ages. Yale University Press.
Everett C (2012). A Closer Look at A
Supposedly Anumeric Language 1. International Journal of American Linguistics,
78(4), 575-590.
Fine K (2012). Mathematics: Discovery
of Invention? Think, 11, pp 11-27
Fitch WT & Friederici AD (2012).
Artificial grammar learning meets formal language theory: an overview. Philosophical
Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 367(1598), 1933–1955.
Accessed 8/20/2015: http://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2012.0103
Hockenbury DH & Hockenbury SE
(2006). Psychology. New York: Worth Publishers.
Kirk GS & Raven JE (1964). The
Presocratic Philosophers, Cambridge University Press.
Lagopoulos, A. P., &
Boklund-Lagopoulou, K. (1992). Meaning and geography: The social conception of
the region in northern Greece (No. 104). Walter de Gruyter.
Ross
KL (2011) Mathematics & Music, after Pythagoras. Accessed 8/20/2015: http://www.friesian.com/music.htm
Stillwell J (2006). Yearning for the
impossible: The surprising truths of mathematics A. K. Peters, Ltd.
I
am indebted to David Edwards, emeritus professor of mathematics from Georgia
University for discussing with me the subtleties of some of these thoughts.
Having such a knowledgeable and challenging adversary promoted the thinking of
this argument and produced a much clearer thesis. However, all misrepresentations,
deficiencies and shortfalls are purely my responsibilities.
© USA Copyrighted 2015 Mario D. Garrett
Tuesday, September 15, 2015
Lost with Dementia
Patterns exist for people who get lost. Whether joggers, hikers, children, or those diagnosed with dementia, different groups exhibit different lost person behaviors. Knowing these patterns would help in finding them. Robert Koester's groundbreaking research has greatly expanded our understanding of “Lost Person Behavior” (Koester, 2008).
This evolving line of inquiry is becoming the bedrock for Search And Rescue (SAR) protocols. SAR personnel consider variables such as behavioral profiles, activity, terrain, health, and personal characteristics to help predict the behavior of people who are declared lost. Koester has analyzed more than 50,000 cases world-wide to identify patterns of behavior that lost people tend to follow, including a special section for people with any variant of dementias—Koester’s specialty.
Older adults with dementia typically get lost through wandering behavior. Six in 10 people with dementia will wander—take off in an aimless way—becoming disoriented and perhaps not remembering their name, address, or their location. Although effective programs exist in the United States—including MedicAlert®, Safe Return® Comfort Zone® and Comfort Zone Check-In®—the majority of people are not enrolled in such electronic tagging. Either they cannot afford the monthly premiums or they assume that they would never need such services. Due to America’s demographic changes, one thing is certain—people with dementia comprise an increasing concern for volunteer SAR teams, as well as state and federal agencies.
For SAR teams all dementias are treated the same. Koester & Stooksbury (1995) reported that when normal older adults got lost they traveled on average a greater straight-line distance (2.56 km) from the Point Last Seen (PLS) than older adults (in this case older than 40 years of age) with dementia (0.88 km). Although statistics indicate greater variability of distance traveled among dementia patients,here are some outliers where some might only travel a short distance while others travel greater distances (one in ten travel an average of 52 miles). Surprisingly, age did not emerge as an important consideration in how people with dementia behave when they get lost.
Dementia patients generally leave their own residence or nursing home and start traveling along roads. The patient is usually located (in 89% of cases) within one mile (1.2 km) of the PLS. If the patients were not on a road (14%), they were usually found in a creek/drainage (28%), and/or caught in briars/bushes (33%).
The majority of patients succumb to the environment. One in five dementia patients (42 cases in this study) were found deceased due to hypothermia, dehydration, or drowning. No fatalities were found among dementia patients when they were located within 24 hours. If they were not found within this 24-hour window, half of dementia patients were likely to be found dead.
Consequently, searches for dementia patients are considered urgent and require an aggressive SAR response. Koester highlights that the diagnosis of dementia is important to elicit urgency. However, he also points out that most primary care physicians fail to administer cognitive status tests, and therefore only correctly identify 58% of the cases as possible dementia. This is particularly true of patients who become lost in wilderness and rural settings, who often belong to disadvantaged socioeconomic groups and who receive minimal health care. Not knowing that the lost person has dementia diminishes the search’s urgency, with potentially detrimental effects.
A key hypothesis among SAR responders when looking for lost dementia patients is referred to as the “path of least resistance.” This theory states that when people with dementia start wandering they tend to follow terrain that provides less resistance (down rather than uphill, roads rather than meandering paths, or railway tracks rather than steps).
A considerable number (28%) of dementia patients were found in drainages or creeks—indicating that they most likely walked downhill—supporting the path of least resistance hypothesis. Another 33% of the patients appear to have become stuck in thick brush or briars. This insight is important because untrained searchers, or searchers working at night often avoid looking into brush or briars because most people tend to try to make themselves visible. Getting stopped in drainages, creeks, brush and briars support research indicating that lost patients with dementia are traveling a path of least resistance until they reach an insurmountable barrier.
Because prevention is the best cure, reducing wandering becomes the best first line of defense. A personal care plan that reduces agitation and depression will help minimize wandering. Because people with dementia tend to leave few clues when they are lost and often do not respond to shouts or voice-based searching, getting expert help as early as possible is crucial. Knowing how to look for lost people with dementia requires an urgency that only expert help can provide.
Local dementia organizations counsel families that when someone with dementia goes missing, SAR efforts should be initiated immediately. Ninety-four percent of people who wander are found within 1.5 miles of where they disappeared. After the family and/or caregivers search the immediate area for no more than 15 minutes, they should call "911" and report to the police that a person with dementia—a vulnerable adult—is missing. Reporting parties should provide enough detail, including any places nearby that might have been important in childhood for the missing person. Reports should include information on fears or phobias that the person might have, and what medications they are on. A Missing Person Report should be filed so that police will also initiate a search.
In addition, even if a concerned party is not enrolled in a locator program, they should file a report with MedicAlert+ Alzheimer's Association Safe Return at 1-800-625-3780. First responders are often trained to check this resource when they locate a missing person with dementia. Additional help can be accessed from the Alzheimer's Association 24/7 Helpline, 1-800-272-3900, which provides information and support to those who need assistance.Getting help as fast as possible might be the strategy that saves the lives of lost people with dementia.
Further Reading:
Perkins D & Roberts P (2000). Search Management for the Initial Response Incident Commander 2000. ERI International Inc.
Twardy CR, Koester R & Gatt R (2006). Missing Person Behaviour An Australian Study Final Report to the Australian National SAR Council. Accessed online: http://www.esf.com.au/documents/reports/RobertGatt2002VicPol.pdf (link is external)
Koester RJ & Stooksbury DE (1999). Behavioral profile of wandering Alzheimer’s patients. Wilderness and Environmental Medicine, 6,34-43 (1995)
Koester RJ (2008) Lost Person Behavior: A Search and Rescue Guide on Where to Look - for Land, Air and Water. Dbs Productions; Spi edition.
© USA Copyrighted 2015 Mario D. Garrett
Subscribe to:
Posts (Atom)